Int J Med Sci 2019; 16(11):1461-1465. doi:10.7150/ijms.35789 This issue Cite

Research Paper

Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample

Bupachad Khanthiyong1,2, Samur Thanoi1,2, Gavin P. Reynolds2,3, Sutisa Nudmamud-Thanoi1,2 Corresponding address

1. Department of Anatomy, Faculty of Medical Science, Naresuan University, Phitsanulok, Thailand
2. Centre of Excellence in Medical Biotechnology, Faculty of Medical Science, Naresuan University, Phitsanulok, Thailand
3. Biomolecular Sciences Research Centre, Sheffield Hallam University, Sheffield, United Kingdom

Citation:
Khanthiyong B, Thanoi S, Reynolds GP, Nudmamud-Thanoi S. Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. Int J Med Sci 2019; 16(11):1461-1465. doi:10.7150/ijms.35789. https://www.medsci.org/v16p1461.htm
Other styles

File import instruction

Abstract

Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) plays a crucial role in the removal of cortical dopamine and is strongly implicated in human executive function. Numerous studies have reported associations of the COMT Val158Met (rs4680) polymorphism with executive function in healthy subjects. However, little work has investigated this in the Thai population and the relationship of age and education with this association remains unclear. Therefore, this study was designed to investigate the association of this polymorphism of the COMT gene with executive cognitive brain function in healthy subjects and the relationship with age and education. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was performed to assess executive function in 254 healthy Thai subjects (aged 20-72 years). The results showed a significant association of rs4680 with executive function, in which Val/Met heterozygotes demonstrated better cognitive set shifting performance. Moreover, Met allele carriers showed a significantly stronger effect in the categories completed score than did Val homozygotes. Furthermore, age and education also showed a significant association with COMT genotype and WCST. These results revealed that executive cognitive function is associated with COMT genotype and influenced by age and/or education level in a Thai sample.

Keywords: Executive function, Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT), Val158Met, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Single Nucleotide Polymorphism (SNP)

Introduction

Executive function is a higher cognitive ability that uses previous experiences and new information to regulate and manage thoughts and actions for successful goal-directed behavior. Executive function processes include planning or organizing, working memory, focus or attention, problem-solving, verbal reasoning, decision-making, cognitive set shifting, self-monitoring and regulation of emotion [1,2]. These complex behaviors are mediated by the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and other brain regions. Currently, various tasks have been used to assess executive function including Trail Making Tests A and B, digit span test, Stroop test, word-fluency test and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). WCST is one of the most popular tasks for measurement of prefrontal cortex function [3].

Dopamine (DA) has been reported to be an important neurotransmitter related to executive function [4]. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) is one enzyme responsible for the degradation of dopamine and regulates the concentration of dopamine, and hence its biological action, in the cortex. Genetic polymorphisms affecting expression or regulation of COMT might therefore influence executive function. A functional single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) of COMT is Val158Met (rs4680) leading to the alteration of enzyme activity; the Met allele produces COMT with a low activity which in turn reduces the degradation rate and increases cortical DA. On the other hand, the higher enzymatic activity of the Val allele results in decreased DA activity [5,6]. At present, numerous studies have reported that this COMT SNP is associated with executive function in healthy subjects, as well as in subjects with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among others [7,8,9]. Numerous studies report that the Met allele is associated with better performance in executive function when compared with the Val allele in healthy subjects [10,11]. Barnett and coworkers [12] identified a relationship of rs4680 with perseverative errors on the WCST in healthy subjects and schizophrenia. However, healthy volunteers, but not schizophrenic patients, with the Met/Met genotype showed better WCST performance. Moreover, the Met/Met genotype, related to higher PFC DA, is associated with the best performance in attention tasks in adult subjects whereas adolescents with Val/Met genotype showed a better attention than Met/Met and Val/Val genotypes [13].However, a meta-analysis has shown no significant effect, although this might relate to differences between samples studied [14]. There also appear to be suggestions of an age effect [15,16], while education level was also reported to be one factor affecting executive ability [17,18]. However, these issues have been little studied in Thailand and remain unclear. Therefore, this study was designed to investigate the association of the rs4680 polymorphism of the COMT gene with executive cognitive brain function in healthy volunteers and the relationship with age and education.

Materials and methods

Subjects

All subjects were Thai aged between 20-72 years and provided written informed consent prior to the study. Level of education was determined and subjects assigned to two groups: those receiving no more than primary education, and those who had received secondary and, for some, tertiary education. Subjects with abnormal mental health evaluated by the Thai Mental Health Indicator (TMHI-55) were excluded from this study. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was also used to exclude subjects with dementia. All experimental protocols in this study were approved by the Human Ethics Committee of Naresuan University (COA No. 553/2017).

Single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) study

The blood sample and DNA extraction was performed using 2 different methods. The blood samples from the cubital vein were collected in EDTA blood collection tube and the genomic DNA was extracted from blood leukocytes by using Trizol LS reagent following by the manufacturing instruction. The fingertip blood samples were collected on FTA cards and the DNA extraction was performed following a previous report [19]. The genotyping of COMT Val158Met SNP (rs4680) was conducted by polymerase chain reaction restriction fragment length polymorphism (PCR-RFLP) method. The amplification of 100 ng DNA template by PCR in a total reaction volume of 25 µl using forward primer 5'-TACTGTGGCTACTCAGCTGTGC-3' and reverse primer 5'-GTGAACGTGGTGTGAACACC-3'[20]. PCR conditions were performed as following: 1) predenaturation at 95°C for 2 min and 45 cycles of denaturation at 95°C for 30 sec, 2) annealing at 65°C for 30 sec, 3) extension at 72°C for 30 sec, and 4) the last cycle of PCR were performed at 72 °C for 5 min. The 236 bp PCR products were digested with restriction enzyme: Hsp92II (promega) and incubated at 37˚C for 2 hr. The complete digestion produces 4 fragments of size 114 bp and/or 96 bp, 54 bp, 44 bp, and 24 bp in which 114 bp represents Val/Val homozygotes, both 114 bp and 96 bp are Val/Met heterozygotes, and 96 bp Val/Val homozygotes. The fragments were separated using 4% agarose gel electrophoresis and visualized by ethidium bromide staining.

Executive function test

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is widely used to test for frontal cortex function in clinical and research contexts [14]. In this study, the subjects were tested by computer-based WCST (Inquisit 3.0.6.0) to assess executive function. Four stimulus cards and 128 response cards were used for the assessment. The response cards contain 3 different dimensions of colors (red, blue, yellow or green), numbers of objects (1,2,3 or 4) and forms (crosses, circles, triangles or stars). The sorting rule is based on color, number, or form but not given to the subject. For this WCST, 4 stimulus cards are shown on the screen of a laptop computer along with a single response card. At the beginning, the instruction of the test was given to subject and subject has to select the correct card matching the response card according to the sorting rule. After matching, the subject is informed of the result (right or wrong). After 4 consecutive correct matches, one completed category, the sorting rule shifts to the next sorting rule without prior warning. This test continues until the subject has either completed 6 categories of 3 different sorting rules or all 128 cards have been used [21].WCST raw score was analyzed and reflected different aspects of executive function as follows [22]:

  • The number of categories completed was determined using the score range between 1 and 6, reflecting cognitive set shifting.
  • Trials to complete the first category was determined the ability to formulate a logical concept with the score range between 0 and 128, reflecting initial conceptualization.
  • The perseverative errors were used to measure the inability to correct the respond due to ignorance of relevant stimuli, reflecting cognitive inflexibility.
  • The percentage of total corrects: the total number of correct response cards multiply 100 and divided by total cards, reflecting initial conceptualization and attention.
  • The percentage of total errors: the total number of incorrect responses cards multiply 100 and divided by total cards, reflecting nonspecific cognitive impairment.

Statistical analysis

SPSS software (IBM SPSS statistics version 23) was used for analyses employing the Pearson Chi-squared test, univariate general linear model, independent t-test, Spearman rank correlation. The significance level was considered at p≤0.05.

Results

Demographic data and effects on genotype

Subjects comprised 110 males and 144 females with a mean age of 46.41±18.32 years (range, 20-72 years). Their demographic data including education, age and sex are described according to genotype in table 1. Age however differed between both sexes (females 43.91±18.80 males 49.68±17.22: t=2.54; p=0.012) and education (t=16.53; p<0.001) categories, although there was no significant relationship between sex and education level (χ2=2.44; p=0.118).

The distribution of genotype shown in table 1 is consistent with proportions expected under Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p=0.078 by χ2 test).No significant difference in age was found between genotypes, although the Val/Val group showed a slightly higher mean age. There were no significant differences in genotype between male and female subjects or in relation to level of education.

Demographic effects on WCST results

All measures except the first category were significantly correlated with age using Spearman rank correlation (Table 2). An effect of educational level on WCST performance was found in % total correct, % total errors and categories completed as shown in table 3. Sex was found to have a significant effect only on the WCST subscale % total correct (p=0.049). Age was included as a covariate in further analyses.

COMT genotype effect on executive function

A significant association between COMT genotype and WCST performance was found for categories completed, in which Val/Met heterozygotes showed improved performance (Table 4). Analysing the results using a two-genotype dominant model (Table 5) showed a somewhat stronger effect in which Met allele carriers showed better performance that Val/Val homozygotes in the categories completed. A recessive model showed no significant differences in any WCST measures (data not shown).

 Table 1 

Demographic data of the three COMT genotypes

Val/Val (N=141)Val/Met (N=89)Met/Met (N=24)p-value
Age47.93±18.1244.54±18.5544.42±18.580.337
Sex (Female/Male)81(57.45%)/60(42.55%)53(59.55%)/36(40.45%)10(41.67%)/14(58.33%)0.281
Educational level
Primary71 (50.35%)39 (43.82%)12 (50%)0.614
Secondary and tertiary70 (49.65%)50 (56.18%)12 (50%)

Data were presented as mean±SD by univariate general linear model.

 Table 2 

The correlation of WCST scores with age in healthy volunteers

WCST (N=254)Correlation
Coefficient
p (2-tailed)
%Total correct-0.1670.007**
%Total error0.2180.008**
1st category completed0.1050.095
Categories completed-0.1340.032*
Perseverative error0.2140.001***

Data were analyzed by Spearman rank correlation. Value were considered significant at *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001.

 Table 3 

Association of WCST scores of healthy volunteers with education level

Educational level
WCST (N=254)Primary (N=122)Secondary/Tertiary (N=132)p-value
% Total correct43.53±12.4948.99±12.490.000***
% Total error56.11±11.5550.97±12.450.001***
1st category completed12.84±12.6111.2±12.840.304
Categories completed4.60±1.575.03±1.510.027*
Perseverative error3.00±4.192.50±5.890.440

Data were presented as mean±SD. *p≤0.05 ***p≤0.001 by univariate general linear model.

 Table 4 

WCST scores of healthy volunteers with genotype of rs4680 in COMT gene

COMT genotype (frequency)Covariate with ageCovariate with education
WCST (N=254)Val/Val (N=141)Val/Met (N=89)Met/Met (N=24)p-valuep-value
% Total correct45.18±12.6348.35±13.1246.00±9.480.2590.240
% Total error54.77±12.6351.17±12.1654.04±9.520.1430.130
1st category completed12.59±13.4210.97±9.8512.25±17.510.7280.677
Categories completed4.58±1.605.13±1.495.08±1.280.034*0.027*
Perseverative error3.18±6.182.50±3.591.04±2.050.1830.170

Data were presented as mean±SD. *p≤0.05 by univariate general linear model as age and education covariate.

 Table 5 

WCST scores of healthy volunteers with Val/Val and Met allele carriers of rs4680 in COMT gene

COMT genotype (frequency)Covariate with ageCovariate with education
WCST (N=254)Val/Val genotype (N=141)Met allele carriers (N=113)p-valuep-value
% Total correct45.18±12.6347.85±12.430.1560.126
% Total error54.77±12.6351.78±11.670.0940.073
1st category completed12.59±13.4211.24±11.810.5070.432
Categories completed4.58±1.605.12±1.450.009**0.007**
Perseverative error3.18±6.182.19±3.370.1730.207

Data were presented as mean±SD. **p≤0.01 by univariate general linear model as age and education covariate.

Discussion

In this study of a sample of healthy subjects from the Thai population, we find an association of measure of cognitive function from the WCST with the rs4680 Val/Met COMT polymorphism, in which carriage of the Met allele is significantly associated with better cognitive set shifting. The study also demonstrated that age and education both influence WCST performance. The two are closely related, reflecting the rapid increase in access to education in Thailand so that more younger people have education beyond a primary level. This relationship makes it difficult to determine which may have a greater influence on cognitive performance as a decline with age is reported [16], as is a relationship with years of education [17,18]. We chose to use age, functioning also as a proxy for the effect of education, as a covariate in statistical analyses of the genetic effect here. The polymorphism was associated with one measures of cognitive function obtained from the WCST. The Val/Met genotype and Met allele carriers demonstrated an increased number of categories completed, which is a measure of cognitive set shifting, one aspect of executive function. There is a strong body of evidence indicating that the Met allele is associated with better executive function than the Val allele, although these findings primarily relate to scores of perseverative errors on the WCST [23,24], which we find not to be significantly changed in our study. However, our results showed a difference in categories completed but not in either total correct or total error which were reported previously [23,24]. Ethnicity may affect the results even though the study has done in the healthy subjects; all subjects in this study are Thai while the study of Malhotra et al. [23] have only 3 Asians. Our study has also assessed both male and female subjects but the study of Caldu et al. [24] was only done in females. One of the factors may affecting on the WCST is age; the mean age of the subjects in our study is different from those previous studies.

It is well-established that rs4680 is functionally related to COMT enzyme activity, in which the highest activity is associated with the Val/Val genotype, and the lowest with Met/Met [5,6]. This results in differences in the neurotransmitter activity of dopamine in the frontal cortex. Furthermore, a recent study has found that COMT Val carriers have a thinner cortex in prefrontal, parietal, and posterior cingulate cortices than COMT Met carriers independent of age, indicating effects on cortical structure, and that genotype and cortical thickness influenced executive function [25].

There are several limitations to our study. In an attempt to obtain a sample approximately representative of the population, the sample covers a range of the population of varying ages and educational background. We have been unable to distinguish the relative and overlapping effects of these two variables on executive function; future studies could address this by selecting a large sample with a more limited age range. Such factors add to the variance in the WCST results and may have contributed to the limited effect of genotype, which did not significantly influence perseverative errors as might be expected. The sample size was not large which limited the opportunity to subdivide the sample further in order, for example, to study the relative effect of sex.

Nevertheless, these findings indicate that executive cognitive function is associated with COMT genotype and influenced by age and/or education level in a healthy Thai sample.

Abbreviations

COMT: catechol-o-methyltransferase; SNP: single nucleotide polymorphism; WCST: wisconsin card sorting test; PCR: polymerase chain reaction; RFLP: restriction fragment length polymorphism; tail making test A, B (TMT-AB); TMHI-66: Thai mental health indicator; MMSE: mini-mental state examination; HWE: Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Acknowledgements

The authors are extremely grateful to all the volunteers for participation and the staff of Nong-Ake Health Promoting Hospital, Nakhon Sawan province, Thailand for data and blood collections. In addition, we would like to express sincere thanks for the Medical Sciences Academic Service Centre Faculty of Medical Science, Naresuan University, Thailand for facility supports through the study. This work was sponsored and supported by the National Research Council of Thailand and partially supported by Naresuan University Research Fund.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interest exists.

References

1. Baddeley AD. Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford university press. 1986

2. Pineda D, Ardila A, Rosselli M, Cadavid C, Mancheno S, Mejia. Executive dysfunctions in child with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Neurosci. 1998;96:177-196

3. Kimberg DY, D'Esposito M, Farah MJ. Frontal lobes: neuropsychological neuropsychological aspects. Feinberg TE, Farah MJ, editors. Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. 1997:409-418

4. Nieoullon A. Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67(1):53-83

5. Blasi G, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Elvevag B, Callicott JH, Das S, Kolachana BS, Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Effect of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met Genotype on Attentional Control. J Neurosci. 2005;25(20):5038-5045

6. Stefanis NC, Van OJ, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN. Effect of COMT Val158Met polymorphism on the Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs Version: tuning rather than improving performance. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(9):1752-1754

7. Sazci A, Ergula E, Kucukalib I, Kilicc G, Kayac G, Karac I. Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val108/158Met polymorphism and susceptibility to schizophrenia: association is more significant in women. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;132(1):51-56

8. Zhao L, Lin Y, Lao G, Wang Y, Guan L, Wei J. Association study of dopamine receptor gene polymorphism with cognitive functions in bipolar I disorder patient. J Affect Disord. 2015;170:85-90

9. Jin J, Liu L, Gao Q, Chan RC, Li H, Chen Y, Wang Y, Qian Q. The divergent impact of COMT Val158Met on executive function in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Genes Brain Behav. 2016;15(2):271-279

10. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Land S. Genetic and vascular modifiers of age-sensitive cognitive skills: effects of COMT, BDNF, ApoE, and hypertension, Neuropsychology. 2009; 23(1):105-116.

11. Bruder GE, Keilp JG, Xu H, Shikhman M, Schori E, Gorman JM, Gilliam TC. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) Genotypes and Working Memory: Associations with Differing Cognitive Operations. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(11):901-907

12. Barnett JH, Jones PB, Robbins TW, Muller U. Effects of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on executive function: a meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(5):502-509

13. Wahlstrom TW, Hooper CJ, Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Oetting WS, Brott MJ, Luciana M. Variations in the Catechol-O- Methyltransferase Polymorphism and Prefrontally Guided Behaviors in Adolescents Dustin. Biol psychiatry. 2007;61(5):626-632

14. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: A meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:180-192

15. Raz N, Dahle CL, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Land S. Effects of age, genes, and pulse pressure on executive functions in healthy adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(6):1124-1137

16. Ashendorf L, McCaffrey RJ. Exploring age-related decline on the Wisconsin card sorting test. The Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;22(2):262-272

17. Lin H, Chan RC, Zheng L, Yang T, Wang Y. Executive functioning in healthy elderly Chinese people. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22(4):501-511

18. Li CL, Hsu HC. Cognitive function and associated factors among older people in Taiwan: Age and sex differences. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;60(1):196-200

19. Iamjan S, Thanoi S. Watiktinkorn P, Nudmamud-Thanoi S, Reynolds GP. BDNF (Val66Met) genetic polymorphism is associated with vulnerability for methamphetamine dependence. Pharmacogenomics. 2015;16(14):1541-1545

20. Solis-Ortiz S, Luque EP, Crespo LM, Munoz MG. Executive functions and selective attention are favored in middle-aged healthy women carriers of the Val/Val genotype of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase gene: a behavioral genetic study. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:67

21. Heaton RK, Chelune GL, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtis G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual: Revised and expanded. North Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources. 1993

22. Rybakowski JK, Borkowska A, Czerski PM, Kapelski P, Dmitrzak-Weglarz M, Hauser J. An association study of dopamine receptors polymorphisms and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in schizophrenia. J Neural Transm. 2005;112(11):1575-1582

23. Malhotra AK, Kestler LJ, Mazzanti C, Bates JA, Goldberg T, Goldman D. A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2002:159 (4); 652- 654

24. Caldu X, Vendrell P, Faz DB, Clemente I, Bargallo N, Jurado MA, Serra-Grabulosa JM, Junque C. Impact of the COMT Val108/158 Met and DAT genotypes on prefrontal function in healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1437-1444

25. Miranda GG, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Frontoparietal cortical thickness mediates the effect of COMT Val158Met polymorphism on age-associated executive function. Neurobiol Aging. 2019; 73: 104-114.

Author contact

Corresponding address Corresponding author: Tel: +6655964672; Fax: +6655964770; E-mail: sutisatac.th


Received 2019-4-16
Accepted 2019-8-16
Published 2019-9-20


Citation styles

APA
Khanthiyong, B., Thanoi, S., Reynolds, G.P., Nudmamud-Thanoi, S. (2019). Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 16(11), 1461-1465. https://doi.org/10.7150/ijms.35789.

ACS
Khanthiyong, B.; Thanoi, S.; Reynolds, G.P.; Nudmamud-Thanoi, S. Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2019, 16 (11), 1461-1465. DOI: 10.7150/ijms.35789.

NLM
Khanthiyong B, Thanoi S, Reynolds GP, Nudmamud-Thanoi S. Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. Int J Med Sci 2019; 16(11):1461-1465. doi:10.7150/ijms.35789. https://www.medsci.org/v16p1461.htm

CSE
Khanthiyong B, Thanoi S, Reynolds GP, Nudmamud-Thanoi S. 2019. Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. Int J Med Sci. 16(11):1461-1465.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). See http://ivyspring.com/terms for full terms and conditions.
Popup Image